Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorWardill, Trevor J.  Concept link
dc.contributor.authorFabian, Samuel T.  Concept link
dc.contributor.authorPettigrew, Ann C.  Concept link
dc.contributor.authorStavenga, Doekele  Concept link
dc.contributor.authorNordström, Karin  Concept link
dc.contributor.authorGonzalez-Bellido, Paloma T.  Concept link
dc.date.accessioned2017-03-22T18:45:36Z
dc.date.available2017-03-22T18:45:36Z
dc.date.issued2017-03-09
dc.identifier.citationCurrent Biology 27 (2017): 854–859en_US
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/1912/8826
dc.description© The Author(s), 2017. This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License. The definitive version was published in Current Biology 27 (2017): 854–859, doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.01.050.en_US
dc.description.abstractOur visual system allows us to rapidly identify and intercept a moving object. When this object is far away, we base the trajectory on the target’s location relative to an external frame of reference [1]. This process forms the basis for the constant bearing angle (CBA) model, a reactive strategy that ensures interception since the bearing angle, formed between the line joining pursuer and target (called the range vector) and an external reference line, is held constant [2; 3 ; 4]. The CBA model may be a fundamental and widespread strategy, as it is also known to explain the interception trajectories of bats and fish [5 ; 6]. Here, we show that the aerial attack of the tiny robber fly Holcocephala fusca is consistent with the CBA model. In addition, Holcocephala fusca displays a novel proactive strategy, termed “lock-on” phase, embedded with the later part of the flight. We found the object detection threshold for this species to be 0.13°, enabled by an extremely specialized, forward pointing fovea (∼5 ommatidia wide, interommatidial angle Δφ = 0.28°, photoreceptor acceptance angle Δρ = 0.27°). This study furthers our understanding of the accurate performance that a miniature brain can achieve in highly demanding sensorimotor tasks and suggests the presence of equivalent mechanisms for target interception across a wide range of taxa.en_US
dc.description.sponsorshipThis work was funded by the Air Force Office of Scientific Research (FA9550-15-1-0188 to P.T.G.-B. and K.N. and FA9550-15-1-0068 to D.G.S.), an Isaac Newton Trust/Wellcome Trust ISSF/University of Cambridge Joint Research Grant (097814/Z/11/Z) to P.T.G.-B., a Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council David Phillips Fellowship (BBSRC, BB/L024667/1) to T.J.W., a Royal Society International Exchange Scheme grant to P.T.G.-B. (75166), a Swedish Research Council grant (2012-4740) to K.N., and a Shared Equipment Grant from the School of Biological Sciences (University of Cambridge, RG70368).en_US
dc.language.isoen_USen_US
dc.publisherCell Pressen_US
dc.relation.urihttps://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.01.050
dc.rightsAttribution 4.0 International*
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/*
dc.titleA novel interception strategy in a miniature robber fly with extreme visual acuityen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.identifier.doi10.1016/j.cub.2017.01.050


Files in this item

Thumbnail
Thumbnail
Thumbnail
Thumbnail
Thumbnail
Thumbnail
Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record

Attribution 4.0 International
Except where otherwise noted, this item's license is described as Attribution 4.0 International